• Bacon, F. T. (1979). Credibility of repeated statements: Memory for trivia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5(3), 241–252. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.5.3.241

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Batchelder, W. H., & Riefer, D. M. (1990). Multinomial processing models of source monitoring. Psychological Review, 97(4), 548–564. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210812

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Bayen, U. J., & Kuhlmann, B. G. (2011). Influences of source-item contingency and schematic knowledge on source monitoring: Tests of the probability-matching account. Journal of Memory and Language, 64(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.09.001

    Article 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Bayen, U. J., Murnane, K., & Erdfelder, E. (1996). Source discrimination, item detection, and multinomial models of source monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(1), 197–215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.1.197

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Bayen, U. J., Nakamura, G. V., Dupuis, S. E., & Yang, C. L. (2000). The use of schematic knowledge about sources in source monitoring. Memory & Cognition, 28(3), 480–500. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198562

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Begg, I. M., Anas, A., & Farinacci, S. (1992). Dissociation of processes in belief: Source recollection, statement familiarity, and the illusion of truth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121(4), 446–458. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.446

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Bell, R., Buchner, A., & Musch, J. (2010). Enhanced old-new recognition and source memory for faces of cooperators and defectors in a social-dilemma game. Cognition, 117(3), 261–275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.020

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Bell, R., Mieth, L., & Buchner, A. (2020). Source attributions for detected new items: Persistent evidence for schematic guessing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(9), 1407–1422. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021820911004

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Bell, R., Mieth, L., & Buchner, A. (2021). Source memory for advertisements: The role of advertising message credibility. Memory & Cognition, 49(1), 32–45. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-020-01075-9

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Britt, S. H., & Adams, S. C. (2007). How many advertisements is a person exposed to in a day? American Association of Advertising Agencies. https://ams.aaaa.org//eweb/upload/FAQs/adexposures.pdf.

  • Bröder, A., & Meiser, T. (2007). Measuring source memory. Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie/journal of Psychology, 215(1), 52–60. https://doi.org/10.1027/0044-3409.215.1.52

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Buchner, A., Bell, R., Mehl, B., & Musch, J. (2009). No enhanced recognition memory, but better source memory for faces of cheaters. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(3), 212–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.01.004

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Calfee, J. E., & Ringold, D. J. (1994). The 70 % majority: Enduring consumer beliefs about advertising. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 13(2), 228–238. https://doi.org/10.1177/074391569401300204

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Cho, C.-H., & Cheon, H. J. (2004). Why do people avoid advertising on the Internet? Journal of Advertising, 33(4), 89–97. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.2004.10639175

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Echterhoff, G., Hirst, W., & Hussy, W. (2005). How eyewitnesses resist misinformation: Social postwarnings and the monitoring of memory characteristics. Memory & Cognition, 33(5), 770–782. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193073

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Epstein, Z., Berinsky, A. J., Cole, R., Gully, A., Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2021). Developing an accuracy-prompt toolkit to reduce COVID-19 misinformation online. Harvard Kennedy School Misinformation Review. https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-71

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Erdfelder, E., Auer, T.-S., Hilbig, B. E., Aßfalg, A., Moshagen, M., & Nadarevic, L. (2009). Multinomial processing tree models. A review of the literature. Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie/journal of Psychology, 217(3), 108–124. https://doi.org/10.1027/0044-3409.217.3.108

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Evans, N. J., & Park, D. (2015). Rethinking the Persuasion Knowledge Model: Schematic antecedents and associative outcomes of persuasion knowledge activation for covert advertising. Journal of Current Issues & Research in Advertising, 36(2), 157–176. https://doi.org/10.1080/10641734.2015.1023873

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Friestad, M., & Wright, P. (1994). The Persuasion Knowledge Model: How people cope with persuasion attempts. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(1), 1–31. https://doi.org/10.1086/209380

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Frost, P., Ingraham, M., & Wilson, B. (2002). Why misinformation is more likely to be recognised over time: A source monitoring account. Memory, 10(3), 179–185. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210143000317

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 701–721. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.79.5.701

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Henkel, L. A., & Mattson, M. E. (2011). Reading is believing: The truth effect and source credibility. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1705–1721. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.018

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Herzog, S., & Hertwig, R. (2013). The ecological validity of fluency. In C. Unkelbach & R. Greifeneder (Eds.), How the fluency of mental processes influences cognition and behavior (pp. 190–219). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203078938

    Chapter 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Hovland, C. I., Janis, I. L., & Kelley, H. H. (1953). Communication and persuasion: Psychological studies of opinion change. Yale University Press.


    Google Scholar
     

  • Hovland, C. I., & Weiss, W. (1951). The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. Public Opinion Quarterly, 15(4), 635–650. https://doi.org/10.1086/266350

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Johnson, M. K. (1997). Source monitoring and memory distortion. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 352(1362), 1733–1745. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1997.0156

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114(1), 3–28. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Keefe, R. S. E., Arnold, M. C., Bayen, U. J., McEvoy, J. P., & Wilson, W. H. (2002). Source-monitoring deficits for self-generated stimuli in schizophrenia: Multinomial modeling of data from three sources. Schizophrenia Research, 57(1), 51–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964%2801%2900306-1

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). Wuggy: A multilingual pseudoword generator. Behavior Research Methods, 42(3), 627–633. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.3.627

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Komar, G. F., Mieth, L., Buchner, A., & Bell, R. Animacy enhances recollection but not familiarity: Convergent evidence from the remember-know-guess paradigm and the process-dissociation procedure. Memory & Cognition. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-022-01339-6(in press).

  • Kroneisen, M., Woehe, L., & Rausch, L. S. (2015). Expectancy effects in source memory: How moving to a bad neighborhood can change your memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(1), 179–189. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0655-9

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Kuhlmann, B. G., Symeonidou, N., Tanyas, H., & Wulff, L. (2021). Remembering and reconstructing episodic context: An overview of source monitoring methods and behavioral findings. In K. D. Federmeier & L. Sahakyan (Eds.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 75, pp. 79–124). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2021.06.002

    Chapter 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Kuhlmann, B. G., Vaterrodt, B., & Bayen, U. J. (2012). Schema bias in source monitoring varies with encoding conditions: Support for a probability-matching account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(5), 1365–1376. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028147

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Law, S. (1995). Belief in imitator claims: The role of source memory. Advances in Consumer Research, 22(1), 165–170.


    Google Scholar
     

  • Law, S. (1998). Do we believe what we remember or do we remember what we believe? Advances in Consumer Research, 25(1), 221–225.


    Google Scholar
     

  • Law, S. (2002). Can repeating a brand claim lead to memory confusion? The effects of claim similarity and concurrent repetition. Journal of Marketing Research, 39(3), 366–378. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.39.3.366.19104

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Leiner, D. J. (2019). SoSci Survey. SoSci Survey GmbH www.soscisurvey.de.

  • Lindsay, D. S., & Johnson, M. K. (1989). The eyewitness suggestibility effect and memory for source. Memory & Cognition, 17(3), 349–358. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198473

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Marsh, R. L., Cook, G. I., & Hicks, J. L. (2006). Gender and orientation stereotypes bias source-monitoring attributions. Memory, 14(2), 148–160. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210544000015

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Meiser, T., & Bröder, A. (2002). Memory for multidimensional source information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(1), 116–137. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.1.116

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Mitchell, J. P., Dodson, C. S., & Schacter, D. L. (2005). fMRI evidence for the role of recollection in suppressing misattribution errors: The illusory truth effect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(5), 800–810. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929053747595

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Mitchell, J. P., Sullivan, A. L., Schacter, D. L., & Budson, A. E. (2006). Misattribution errors in alzheimer’s disease: The illusory truth effect. Neuropsychology, 20(2), 185–192. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.20.2.185

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Mitchell, K. J., & Johnson, M. K. (2009). Source monitoring 15 years later: What have we learned from fMRI about the neural mechanisms of source memory? Psychological Bulletin, 135(4), 638–677. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015849

    Article 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Moshagen, M. (2010). multiTree: A computer program for the analysis of multinomial processing tree models. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 42–54. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.1.42

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Nadarevic, L., & Erdfelder, E. (2013). Spinoza’s error: Memory for truth and falsity. Memory & Cognition, 41(2), 176–186. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0251-z

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Nadarevic, L., & Erdfelder, E. (2019). More evidence against the Spinozan model: Cognitive load diminishes memory for “true” feedback. Memory & Cognition, 47(7), 1386–1400. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-019-00940-6

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), aac4716. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Pena, M. M., Klemfuss, J. Z., Loftus, E. F., & Mindthoff, A. (2017). The effects of exposure to differing amounts of misinformation and source credibility perception on source monitoring and memory accuracy. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 4(4), 337–347. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000137

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Pennycook, G., Epstein, Z., Mosleh, M., Arechar, A. A., Eckles, D., & Rand, D. G. (2021). Shifting attention to accuracy can reduce misinformation online. Nature, 592(7855), 590–595. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03344-2

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Pennycook, G., McPhetres, J., Zhang, Y., Lu, J. G., & Rand, D. G. (2020). Fighting COVID-19 misinformation on social media: Experimental evidence for a scalable accuracy-nudge intervention. Psychological Science, 31(7), 770–780. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620939054

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Pornpitakpan, C. (2004). The persuasiveness of source credibility: A critical review of five decades’ evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34(2), 243–281. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb02547.x

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Rapp, D. N., Hinze, S. R., Kohlhepp, K., & Ryskin, R. A. (2014). Reducing reliance on inaccurate information. Memory & Cognition, 42(1), 11–26. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0339-0

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Roggeveen, A. L., & Johar, G. V. (2002). Perceived source variability versus familiarity: Testing competing explanations for the truth effect. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(2), 81–91. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327663JCP1202_02

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Schaper, M. L., Kuhlmann, B. G., & Bayen, U. J. (2019a). Metamemory expectancy illusion and schema-consistent guessing in source monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(3), 470–496. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000602

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Schaper, M. L., Mieth, L., & Bell, R. (2019b). Adaptive memory: Source memory is positively associated with adaptive social decision making. Cognition, 186, 7–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.01.014

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Schütz, J., & Bröder, A. (2011). Signal detection and threshold models of source memory. Experimental Psychology, 58(4), 293–311. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000097

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Skurnik, I., Yoon, C., Park, D., & Schwarz, N. (2005). How warnings about false claims become recommendations. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(4), 713–724. https://doi.org/10.1086/426605

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Snodgrass, J. G., & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117(1), 34–50. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.117.1.34

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Spaniol, J., & Bayen, U. J. (2002). When is schematic knowledge used in source monitoring? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(4), 631–651. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.4.631

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Twenge, J. M., Martin, G. N., & Spitzberg, B. H. (2019). Trends in U.S. adolescents’ media use, 1976–2016: The rise of digital media, the decline of TV, and the (near) demise of print. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 8(4), 329–345. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000203

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Unkelbach, C., & Stahl, C. (2009). A multinomial modeling approach to dissociate different components of the truth effect. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(1), 22–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.09.006

    Article 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Van der Schuur, W. A., Baumgartner, S. E., Sumter, S. R., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2015). The consequences of media multitasking for youth: A review. Computers in Human Behavior, 53, 204–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.06.035

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Watkins, L., Aitken, R., Gage, R., Smith, M. B., Chambers, T. J., Barr, M., Stanley, J., & Signal, L. N. (2019). Capturing the commercial world of children: The feasibility of wearable cameras to assess marketing exposure. The Journal of Consumer Affairs, 53(4), 1396–1420. https://doi.org/10.1111/joca.12234

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Yeykelis, L., Cummings, J. J., & Reeves, B. (2014). Multitasking on a single device: Arousal and the frequency, anticipation, and prediction of switching between media content on a computer. Journal of Communication, 64(1), 167–192. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12070

    Article 

    Google Scholar
     

  • Rights and permissions

    Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

    Disclaimer:

    This article is autogenerated using RSS feeds and has not been created or edited by OA JF.

    Click here for Source link (https://www.springeropen.com/)

    Loading